wp-plugin-hostgator
domain was triggered too early. This is usually an indicator for some code in the plugin or theme running too early. Translations should be loaded at the init
action or later. Please see Debugging in WordPress for more information. (This message was added in version 6.7.0.) in /home4/scienrds/scienceandnerds/wp-includes/functions.php on line 6114ol-scrapes
domain was triggered too early. This is usually an indicator for some code in the plugin or theme running too early. Translations should be loaded at the init
action or later. Please see Debugging in WordPress for more information. (This message was added in version 6.7.0.) in /home4/scienrds/scienceandnerds/wp-includes/functions.php on line 6114Source: https:\/\/www.theverge.com\/2022\/7\/27\/23280462\/video-game-mental-health-animal-crossing-well-being<\/a> Video games aren\u2019t hurting people\u2019s mental health, and they\u2019re not helping it. In fact, they don\u2019t do much to move the needle at all, according to a new study<\/a> of tens of thousands of gamers. <\/p>\n For years, policymakers and public health bodies have expressed concern<\/a> about the potential for video games to be addictive or harm mental health. This study, published in the journal Royal Society Open Science,<\/em> offers one of the more comprehensive looks at the relationship between video games and well-being. It builds on previous research<\/a> from the same team that also didn\u2019t find bad effects on mental health. <\/p>\n The research team worked with video game publishers to recruit nearly 39,000 people <\/strong>who played one of seven games: Animal Crossing: New Horizons<\/em>, Apex Legends<\/em>, Eve Online<\/em>, Forza Horizon 4<\/em>, Gran Turismo Sport<\/em>, and The Crew 2<\/em>. The game publishers provided game play data for participants over six weeks, and the researchers surveyed participants three times. <\/p>\n Because the team was able to examine player\u2019s gaming data, they didn\u2019t have to rely on player\u2019s self-reporting the amount of time they spent playing games \u2014 so the team was able to get a more accurate read on gaming time. The study measured well-being using two tools: the scale of positive and negative experiences, which asks people to rank how often they experienced feelings like \u201chappy\u201d and \u201cafraid,\u201d and the Cantril self-anchoring scale, which asks people to say where they are on a ladder with the top representing their best possible life. <\/p>\n The study also asked people to take the Player Experience of Need Satisfaction survey, which tracks people\u2019s experience with specific games \u2014 tracking things like their perception of autonomy and their motivations to play the game. <\/p>\n The analysis found that spending more or less time playing games didn\u2019t have a negative or positive impact on how people felt. Conversely, how people felt didn\u2019t have a major impact on how much time people spent gaming. <\/p>\n Any role video games play in skewing well-being that did pop up in the study was too small to have a real-world impact on how people feel, the authors said. People would have to play games for 10 more<\/em> hours per day than their baseline to notice changes in their well-being, the study found. <\/p>\n The study did find some evidence, though, that people\u2019s motivations to play games and their experience playing them had a slightly larger impact on well-being. When people played games because they wanted to, their well-being was better than when people played games because they felt compelled to. Still, those relationships were small, and it\u2019s not clear if those motivations would have much of a noticeable impact for players. <\/p>\n There\u2019s still more to learn about the ways video games impact how people feel and how they behave, the study authors noted. This analysis only looked at a handful of the thousands of games on the market. Researchers still need to scrutinize how motivations to play games and quality of gameplay could shift people\u2019s experiences. They also need to figure out if certain people have characteristics that make them more or less susceptible to shifts in well-being. <\/p>\n \u201cWe know we need much more player data from many more platforms to develop the kind of deeper understanding required to inform policy and shape advice to parents and medical professionals,\u201d said study author Andrew Przybylski, a senior research fellow at the Oxford Internet Institute, in a statement. <\/p>\n<\/div>\n
\n
<\/br><\/code><\/p>\n